News Release

Avoiding stumbles, from spacewalks to sidewalks

Vibrating footwear could help astronauts and visually impaired earthlings skirt obstacles

Peer-Reviewed Publication

Massachusetts Institute of Technology

Vibrating Footwear

image: Researchers are developing a new boot with built-in sensors and tiny "haptic" motors, whose vibrations can guide the wearer around or over obstacles. Vibrations will jump from low to high intensity when the wearer is at risk of colliding with an obstacle. view more 

Credit: Illustration: Jose-Luis Olivares/MIT

Video of astronauts tripping over moon rocks can make for entertaining Internet viewing, but falls in space can jeopardize astronauts' missions and even their lives. Getting to one's feet in a bulky, pressurized spacesuit can consume time and precious oxygen reserves, and falls increase the risk that the suit will be punctured.

Most falls happen because spacesuits limit astronauts' ability to both see and feel the terrain around them, so researchers from MIT's Department of Aeronautics and Astronautics (AeroAstro) and the Charles Stark Draper Laboratory in Cambridge, Massachusetts are developing a new space boot with built-in sensors and tiny "haptic" motors, whose vibrations can guide the wearer around or over obstacles.

At the International Conference on Human-Computer Interaction, the researchers presented the results of a preliminary study designed to determine what types of stimuli, administered to what parts of the foot, could provide the best navigation cues. On the basis of that study, they're planning further trials using a prototype of the boot.

The work could also have applications in the design of navigation systems for the visually impaired. The development of such systems has been hampered by a lack of efficient and reliable means of communicating spatial information to users.

"A lot of students in my lab are looking at this question of how you map wearable-sensor information to a visual display, or a tactile display, or an auditory display, in a way that can be understood by a nonexpert in sensor technologies," says Leia Stirling, an assistant professor of AeroAstro and an associate faculty member at MIT's Institute for Medical Engineering and Science, whose group led the work. "This initial pilot study allowed Alison [Gibson, a graduate student in AeroAstro and first author on the paper] to learn about how she could create a language for that mapping." Gibson and Stirling are joined on the paper by Andrea Webb, a psychophysiologist at Draper.

What, where, and when

For the pilot study, Gibson developed a device that spaced six haptic motors around each of a subject's feet -- one motor each at the heel, big toe, and instep, and three motors along the outer edge of the foot. The intensity of the motors' vibrations could be varied continuously between minimum and maximum settings.

A subject placed his or her feet in the device while seated before a computer. Software asked the subjects to indicate when they felt vibrations and at what locations on the foot. Tests were conducted under two conditions. In the first, the subjects focused on the stimuli to their feet. In the second, they were distracted by a simple cognitive test: The software would flash a random number on the screen, and the subject would count upward from that number by threes. The vibration of one of the motors would interrupt the counting, and the subject would report on the sensation.

Each subject was asked to report on more than 500 individual stimuli, divided between the two conditions.

The researchers had envisioned that variations in the intensity of the motors' vibrations could indicate distance to obstacles, as measured by sensors built into the boot. But they found that when distracted by cognitive tests, subjects had difficulty identifying steady increases in intensity. And even when they were attending to the stimuli, the subjects still had difficulty identifying decreases in intensity.

Subjects also had difficulty distinguishing between the locations of stimuli on the outer edge of the foot. Strangely, in 20 percent of cases, distributed across all study participants, subjects were entirely unable to discern low-intensity stimuli to the middle location on the outer edge of the right foot.

Boot-building

On the basis of the study results, Gibson is developing a boot with motors at only three locations: at the toe, at the heel, and toward the front of the outside of the foot -- away from the middle location where stimuli sometimes didn't register.

Stimuli will not be varied continuously, but they will jump from low to high intensity when the wearer is at risk of colliding with an obstacle. The high-intensity stimuli will also be pulsed, to help distinguish them from the low-intensity ones.

In principle, the motor at the side of the foot could help guide the user around obstacles, but the first trial of the boot will concentrate entirely on the problem of stepping over obstacles of different heights. The researchers will also be evaluating the haptic signals in conjunction with, and separately from, visual signals, to determine the optimal method of conveying spatial information.

###


Disclaimer: AAAS and EurekAlert! are not responsible for the accuracy of news releases posted to EurekAlert! by contributing institutions or for the use of any information through the EurekAlert system.