News Release

How does the mind grasp climate change?

A research-based guide tries to narrow a communication gap

Book Announcement

Columbia Climate School

Eeek Factor

image: People tend to react to threats that seem immediate and ignore ones that seem abstract, no matter how large they are. view more 

Credit: Illustration by Ian Webster

A recent poll shows that the proportion of Americans who are convinced that human activity is warming Earth's climate has dropped sharply since last year, to under 40 percent--even though scientists say the data is overwhelming, and continues to build rapidly. A concise new publication delves into what goes on in the human mind that causes this disconnect, and what communicators of climate science can do about it.

The new 43-page guide, The Psychology of Climate Change Communication, released today by Columbia University's Center for Research on Environmental Decisions, looks at how people process information and decide to take action, or not. Using research into groups as disparate as African farmers and conservative U.S. voters, it offers insights on how scientists, journalists and educators can convey evolving scientific knowledge, and increase chances that the public will understand them, and take action when appropriate.

For the nonscientist, climate can seem alternately confusing, overwhelming and politically loaded, say lead authors Debika Shome and Sabine Marx. In eight brief chapters, the guide identifies basic tactics to overcome predictable roadblocks. For one, communicators would do well to frame complex issues in ways that people can relate to personally: New Yorkers may respond more to the idea that sea-level rise threatens to flood their subways, than to the idea that it also threatens much of Bangladesh. Communicators also need to do a better job of sorting the larger picture from smaller uncertainties—for instance, concentrating on the strong consensus that sea levels will rise in the 21st century, versus confusing readers with disagreements over exactly how much levels will rise.

Scientists generally acknowledge that nothing can be known with absolute certainty; their trade involves reducing the amount of uncertainty. But, as with the numbers they give out, the words they habitually use can be misinterpreted to mean they do not really know what they are talking about. For instance, a recent report from the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change states that global temperature increases that have taken place in the last 50 years have been "very likely due to the observed increase in anthropogenic GHG concentrations." Panel scientists have agreed that "very likely" means 90 percent certain or more--but when researchers asked ordinary people to assign a percentage to that specific phrase, most came up with a much lower number. The guide also attacks fancy words like anthropogenic (translation: manmade) and acronyms such as GHG, (greenhouse gases), which may alienate even educated people. Even many graphs that in the eyes of scientists show alarming trends elicit only yawns or incomprehension from almost everyone else.

One chart in the guide lists words with columns showing their meaning as perceived by scientists, and by nonscientists. To scientists, a "theory" is the "physical understanding of how [something] works." Hence, the well-accepted theory of evolution, the theory that the earth formed over billions of years—and now, the theory of manmade climate change. But to the public, a theory may be just "a hunch, conjecture or speculation." (Politicians long ago learned the lesson that language matters: one recent study by the authors and their colleagues finds that conservative Americans find "carbon offsets" more acceptable than a "carbon tax"—even though it might be argued the two are essentially the same. Climate legislation now before Congress avoids mention of anything labeled "tax.")

The public has its own chronic problems. For one thing, there is a phenomenon that social scientists call the "finite pool of worry." In other words, people can deal with only so much bad news at a time before they tune out. For another, when individuals respond to threats like climate change, they are likely to alleviate their worries by taking only one action, even if it is in their interest to take more than one—an effect called "single action bias." For Americans, recycling often serves as a catchall green measure; some people will zealously separate out their trash, but neglect to do anything more to curb high energy usage in their homes or vehicles. One study showed that farmers in Argentina who had capacity to store grain in the face of drought were less likely to use irrigation or crop insurance, even though those added measures would have made their operations more resilient to weather.

"Gaining public support for climate change policies and encouraging environmentally responsible behavior depends on a clear understanding of how people process information and make decisions," say Shome and Marx. "Social science provides an essential part of the puzzle."

###

Free printed copies and an interactive online version of the guide are available from the Center for Research on Environmental Decisions. The project received funding from the Charles Evans Hughes Memorial Foundation and the U.S. National Science Foundation.

Copies of the guide: http://cred.columbia.edu/guide/home.html

Author contacts:

Sabine Marx sm2234@columbia.edu 212-854-8760
Debika Shome ds2382@columbia.edu

The Earth Institute, Columbia University mobilizes the sciences, education and public policy to achieve a sustainable earth. Through interdisciplinary research among more than 500 scientists in diverse fields, the Institute is adding to the knowledge necessary for addressing the challenges of the 21st century and beyond. With over two dozen associated degree curricula and a vibrant fellowship program, the Earth Institute is educating new leaders to become professionals and scholars in the growing field of sustainable development. We work alongside governments, businesses, nonprofit organizations and individuals to devise innovative strategies to protect the future of our planet. www.earth.columbia.edu

The Center for Research on Environmental Decisions is an interdisciplinary center that studies individual and group decision making under climate uncertainty and in the face of environmental risk. CRED's objectives address the human responses to climate change and climate variability as well as improved communication and increased use of scientific information on climate variability and change. Located at Columbia University, it is affiliated with The Earth Institute and the Institute for Social and Economic Research and Policy. http://www.cred.columbia.edu/


Disclaimer: AAAS and EurekAlert! are not responsible for the accuracy of news releases posted to EurekAlert! by contributing institutions or for the use of any information through the EurekAlert system.