News Release

Using the outside world to save on brainpower

Peer-Reviewed Publication

Cell Press

Metacognitive Strategies

image: This illustration depicts metacognitive evaluation of internal vs. external strategies. view more 

Credit: Evan F. Risko

Every day, we rely on our physical surroundings--friends, gadgets, and even hand gestures--to manage incoming information and retain it. In a Review published August 16 in Trends in Cognitive Sciences, two researchers explain the myriad ways in which forms of assistance from gestures to GPS affect both what we know and what we think we know.

Evan F. Risko, a Canada Research chair in Cognitive Psychology at the University of Waterloo, and co-author Sam Gilbert, a Royal Society research fellow at University College London, call these behaviors "cognitive offloading"--physical actions that reduce the mental effort needed to perform a task. When required to remember an appointment, for example, people are faced with the choice of internally remembering it or "offloading" it, by writing it down in a calendar or setting a reminder with a smartphone. Similarly, your accountant may choose to use a calculator when going over your finances rather than mentally perform all of the necessary computations.

What's happening here, say the authors, is the use of a physical action (writing, using a calculator) to eliminate the need for that internal effort--the cognitive burden. One of the key messages of Risko and Gilbert's Review is that people will engage in such offloading behavior when they believe it is more efficient than their internal abilities. However, these beliefs do not always match reality.

"The growing interaction between people and technology has really brought interest in the subject to the forefront. People want to understand how technology affects the way we think," says Risko. Gilbert adds, "Cognitive offloading undoubtedly brings huge benefits, but also potential costs. We are just beginning to understand these effects. For example, how can technology allow us to remain independent as we grow older, and what might the downsides be to relying on external devices?"

Though some people may think of offloading as harmful to one's ability to remember, the truth is that these strategies are needed for everyday thinking and planning. People will usually turn toward these offloading strategies because we are "capacity limited" in our cognitive abilities. We may be able to remember a list of five, six, or seven numbers, but it's unlikely that we could remember 47 of them. Offloading gives us the ability to "subvert our cognitive limits," say the researchers, who point out that "devices such as smartphones allow us to be more confident and able to do things that we've not been able to do before."

However, offloading can also have negative consequences. "If you're allowed to store some to-be-remembered information on a computer, chances are you won't devote cognitive real estate to remembering it," says Risko. "As a result, your ability to remember that information without the computer will likely be reduced. There's little doubt that these new technologies are affecting what we remember."

Recent research has demonstrated some of the consequences of offloading, particularly when offloading onto electronic devices, such as personal computers or digital cameras. In one study (doi: 10.1177/0956797613504438), researchers gave museumgoers digital cameras and found that taking photographs degraded subjects' ability to remember the objects they viewed, and specific details about those objects, when compared to their ability to remember objects not photographed. Another study, by Gilbert (doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.021), showed that using reminders changed the physical activity inside volunteers' brains when they remembered plans and intentions.

Moving forward, Risko and Gilbert want to focus on the way offloading changes the way we think, as well as what's happening in the brain as a result of different offloading strategies. They are also interested in the long-term consequences of living in a modern, high-tech environment that allows us to constantly offload our cognition.

Risko stressed that these are all questions that can only be answered with time. "There's a lot of conversation about whether devices like smartphones ar¬e ruining us cognitively or not," says Risko. "There isn't a lot of research available yet that addresses the long-term cognitive consequences of offloading. What our Review has shown is that similar scientific principles apply to our use of a wide variety of devices, such as pen and paper, GPS trackers, and smartphones. This should make it easier for researchers to fully understand these devices' consequences in the future."

###

Evan F. Risko was supported by funding from the Canada Research Chairs program, the Natural Science and Engineering Research Council, and an Early Researcher Award from the Province of Ontario. Sam Gilbert is supported by a Royal Society University Research Fellowship.

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Risko and Gilbert: "Cognitive Offloading" http://www.cell.com/trends/cognitive-sciences/fulltext/S1364-6613(16)30098-5

Trends in Cognitive Sciences (@TrendsCognSci), published by Cell Press, is a monthly review journal that brings together research in psychology, artificial intelligence, linguistics, philosophy, computer science, and neuroscience. It provides a platform for the interaction of these disciplines and the evolution of cognitive science as an independent field of study. Visit http://www.cell.com/trends/cognitive-sciences. To receive Cell Press media alerts, please contact press@cell.com.


Disclaimer: AAAS and EurekAlert! are not responsible for the accuracy of news releases posted to EurekAlert! by contributing institutions or for the use of any information through the EurekAlert system.