News Release

How reciprocity can magnify inequality

Peer-Reviewed Publication

Association for Psychological Science

People tend to reciprocate others' actions in ways that increase disparities in wealth, according to findings published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

"People typically want to treat others fairly, repaying kindness with kindness. Yet, we find that people reciprocate a little more with wealthy individuals, even when those wealthy individuals display equivalent generosity to less wealthy ones," explains psychological scientist Leor Hackel of Stanford University, first author on the research. "In other words, people see kindness through wealth-tinted glasses, and sometimes repay the most to those who need it least."

Rising economic inequality has become a pressing issue around the world. Hackel and Stanford colleague Jamil Zaki were specifically interested in understanding how inequality in social capital--the resources that people have access to through their relationships with others--might emerge.

Social capital is critical to navigating the modern world and we rely on it whenever we ask friends to help us move, recommend job opportunities, or loan us money. It seems plausible that our desire to reciprocate others' generosity would lead to decreased inequality. But Hackel and Zaki hypothesized that our tendency to focus on the amount, or reward value, that people give would disproportionately benefit the wealthy, widening the wealth gap over the long term.

In one experiment, the researchers randomly assigned points to online participants, which would later be converted to cash. Some of these participants were relatively wealthy, receiving a total of 200 points; others were less wealthy, receiving only 60 points. As "givers," these participants could repeatedly choose to share either 20% or 50% of their wealth with a future participant.

A separate group of "recipients" then made a series of decisions, choosing which giver they wanted to interact with. In each round, recipients saw the number of points that giver had available in that round and how much the giver had shared.

In a subsequent surprise task, recipients learned they could actually share points in return with each giver. They had an equal number of points to share with each giver, so there was no strategic reason to favor wealthy givers.

In line with previous research, participants shared a larger percentage of points with givers were typically generous (i.e., those who shared half of their wealth) as opposed to those who were less generous.

But they also shared more with givers who had greater wealth. As a result, givers who started with more points also accumulated more compared with less-wealthy givers, even when they offered an equally generous percentage of their wealth.

And recipients favored the wealthy even when there was no potential benefit to them. In a second experiment, recipients rated givers in lieu of reciprocating their offers. These ratings, from 1 to 5 stars, would supposedly help future "investors" make decisions about whom to trust.

The data revealed that wealthier givers received more stars than their less-wealthy peers even when both groups were equally generous. When Hackel and Zaki looked at ratings for givers who never chose the generous 50% allocation, they found that wealthier givers still had a .8 star advantage, on average, relative to less-wealthy givers.

These reputational ratings intensified the wealth gap, as investors chose to interact with the highest-rated giver 93% of the time.

The researchers found that a reinforcement learning model - a model in which learning is based primarily on reward values - accounted for this pattern of findings.

Together, these findings show how differences in social capital can increase over time.

"Our hope is to understand how these changes might ripple across society," Hackel says. "If people privilege the wealthy in their individual interactions, how does that impact the way in which members of a society cooperate with or trust one another as a whole? And how can individuals with fewer resources overcome these barriers and build more equal social capital?"

###

This work was funded by the U.S. National Science Foundation (Award 1606959).

All data and materials have been made publicly available via the Open Science Framework. The complete Open Practices Disclosure for this article is available online. This article has received badges for Open Data and Open Materials.

For more information about this study, please contact: Leor Hackel at lhackel@stanford.edu.

The article abstract is available online at http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0956797617741720

The APS journal Psychological Science is the highest ranked empirical journal in psychology. For a copy of the article "Propagation of Economic Inequality Through Reciprocity and Reputation" and access to other Psychological Science research findings, please contact Anna Mikulak at 202-293-9300 or amikulak@psychologicalscience.org.


Disclaimer: AAAS and EurekAlert! are not responsible for the accuracy of news releases posted to EurekAlert! by contributing institutions or for the use of any information through the EurekAlert system.