News Release

Chimpanzee lethal aggression a result of adaptation rather than human impacts

Peer-Reviewed Publication

Arizona State University

Ian Gilby in the Field with Chimpanzees

image: This image depicts Ian Gilby in the field with chimpanzees. view more 

Credit: Ian Gilby

In the 1970s, Jane Goodall's reports of chimpanzee violence caught the attention of a global audience. Since then, many people have compared chimpanzee intergroup aggression to primitive warfare and have argued that chimpanzee violence is an adaptive strategy that gives the perpetrators an edge. Others have argued that lethal aggression is the consequence of human activities such as provisioning (artificial feeding) by researchers or habitat destruction. A new study of the pattern of intergroup aggression in chimpanzees and their close relatives, bonobos (also called pygmy chimpanzees), finds that human impact isn't the culprit.

"This study debunks the idea that lethal aggression among wild chimpanzees is an aberrant behavior caused by human disturbances, like artificial feeding or habitat loss," says ASU anthropologist Ian Gilby. Gilby is codirector of the long-term Gombe chimpanzee database for the Jane Goodall Institute Research Center and a coauthor on the results published in the journal Nature on September 18.

The research project compiled data collected over five decades from 18 chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and four bonobo (Pan paniscus) communities.

This study represents the first effort to test the human impact versus adaptive strategies hypothesis, which views killing as an evolved tactic to increase access to territory, food, mates or other benefits. Analysis of 152 lethal attacks finds that it is clear that lethal aggression directed toward members of other groups is part of the natural behavioral repertoire of chimpanzees—sometimes chimpanzees kill each other, regardless of human impact. However, bonobos were not observed to kill other bonobos, whatever the level of human impact.

"Gombe is longest running of wild chimpanzee study sites," says Gilby, "and is critically important for addressing questions about the impact of 'provisioning.'" The practice of provisioning was discontinued at Gombe many years ago due to risk of infecting chimpanzees with human diseases.

Concurrent with the published study in Nature, Joan Silk, an ASU scientist who studies primate behavioral ecology, evolution and sociality, wrote a companion piece, "The evolutionary roots of lethal conflict" for the publication's "News and Views" column. In it, Silk points out that the debates about the origins and prevalence of human warfare may be echoed in the search for the answer to chimpanzee adaptation versus human disturbance question. In the article, she says that "perceptions of behavior of primates, particularly chimpanzees, are often distorted" so that "morally desirable features, such as empathy and altruism, have deep evolutionary roots, whereas undesirable features, such as group-level violence and sexual coercion, do not."

The data, however, she says, show that "there are some circumstances in which the benefits of lethal aggression exceed the costs for chimpanzees, nothing more. Humans are not destined to be warlike because chimpanzees sometimes kill their neighbors."

###

Gilby, an assistant professor in ASU's School of Human Evolution and Social Change, studies chimpanzee behavioral ecology at Gombe, focusing on male cooperation and social bonds. Silk is a professor in the School of Human Evolution and Social Change. Both Gilby and Silk are research affiliates with ASU's Institute of Human Origins.


Disclaimer: AAAS and EurekAlert! are not responsible for the accuracy of news releases posted to EurekAlert! by contributing institutions or for the use of any information through the EurekAlert system.