News Release

Seals have a sense of rhythm

Peer-Reviewed Publication

Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics

The capacity to perceive rhythm and to produce novel vocalizations are crucial for human speech and music. Do other mammals possess these capacities?

image: The capacity to perceive rhythm and to produce novel vocalizations are crucial for human speech and music. Do other mammals possess these capacities? Researchers tested rhythm processing in seals; like humans, seals learn vocalizations. It was found that, spontaneously and without training, young seals perceive the rhythmicity of other seals’ vocalizations and discriminate between more vs. less rhythmic sound sequences. These results show that another mammal, apart from us, shows rhythm processing and vocalization learning and suggest that these two skills coevolved in both humans and seals. view more 

Credit: Laura Verga

Why are we such chatty, musical animals? Evolutionary biologists think that our capacities for speech and music may be linked: only animals that can learn new vocalisations—such as humans and songbirds—seem to have a sense of rhythm. “We know that our closest relatives, non-human primates, need to be trained to respond to rhythm”, explains first author Laura Verga. “And even when trained, primates show very different rhythmic capacities to ours”. But what about other mammals?

Seal rhythm

The researchers decided to test the rhythmic abilities of harbour seals, animals known to be capable of vocal learning. The team first created sequences of seal vocalisations. The sequences differed in three rhythmic properties: tempo (fast or slow, like beats per minute in music), length (short or long, like duration of musical notes) and regularity (regular or irregular, like a metronome vs. the rhythm of free jazz). Would infant seals react to these rhythmic patterns?

 

The team tested twenty young seals, held at a rehabilitation centre (the Dutch Sealcentre Pieterburen) before being released into the wild. Using a method from human infant studies, the team recorded how many times the seals turned their head to look at the sound source (behind their backs). Such looking behaviour indicates whether animals (or infants) find a stimulus interesting. If seals can discriminate between different rhythmic properties, they might look longer or more often when they hear a sequence they prefer.

 

The seals looked more often when vocalisations were longer, faster, or rhythmically regular. This means that the 1-year-old seals—without training or rewards—spontaneously discriminated between regular (metronomic) and irregular (arrhythmic) sequences, sequences with short vs. long notes, and sequences with fast vs. slow-paced tempo.

Evolutionary origins

 “Another mammal, apart from us, shows rhythm processing and vocalisation learning”,  says Verga. “This is a significant advance in the debate over the evolutionary origins of human speech and musicality, which are still rather mysterious. Similarly to human babies, the rhythm perception we find in seals arises early in life, is robust and requires neither training nor reinforcement.”

Next, Verga and her team want to find out whether seals perceive rhythm in vocalisations of other animals, or even abstract sounds. And whether other mammals show the same skills: “Are seals special, or are other mammals also capable of spontaneously perceiving rhythm?”

Publication

Laura Verga, Marlene Sroka, Mila Varola, Stella Villanueva & Andrea Ravignani (2022). Spontaneous rhythm discrimination in a mammalian vocal learner. Biology Letters, 20220316.

 

 


Disclaimer: AAAS and EurekAlert! are not responsible for the accuracy of news releases posted to EurekAlert! by contributing institutions or for the use of any information through the EurekAlert system.